Thursday, December 27, 2018

CARCINOMA OF THE STOMACH STAGING CLASSIFICATION

Source: American Joint Committee on Cancer (AJCC), AJCC Cancer Staging Handbook, 7th edition.

CARCINOMA OF THE STOMACH STAGING CLASSIFICATION

Carcinoma in situ: intraepithelial tumor without invasion of the lamina propria
IA
Tumor invades lamina propria, muscularis mucosae, or submucosa without lymph node involvement or metastasis
IB
Tumor invades muscularis propria without lymph node involvement or metastasisor tumor invades lamina propria, muscularis mucosae, or submucosa with metastasis in 1–2 regional lymph nodes
IIA
Tumor penetrates subserosal connective tissue without invasion of visceral peritoneum or adjacent structures and without lymph node involvement or metastasisor tumor invades muscularis propria with metastasis in 1–2 regional lymph nodes; or tumor invades lamina propria, muscularis mucosae, or submucosa with metastasis in 3–6 regional lymph nodes
IIB
Tumor invades serosa (visceral peritoneum) without lymph node involvement or metastasisor tumor penetrates subserosal connective tissue without invasion of visceral peritoneum or adjacent structures with metastasis in 1–2 regional lymph nodes; or tumor invades muscularis propria with metastasis in 3–6 regional lymph nodes; or tumorinvades lamina propria, muscularis mucosae, or submucosa with metastasis in 7 or more regional lymph nodes
IIIA
Tumor invades serosa (visceral peritoneum) with metastasis in 1–2 regional lymph nodes; or tumor penetrates subserosal connective tissue without invasion of visceral peritoneum or adjacent structures with metastasis in 3–6 regional lymph nodes; or tumor invades muscularis propria with metastasis in 7 or more regional lymph nodes
IIIB
Tumor invades adjacent structures with metastasis in 0–2 regional lymph nodes; or tumor invades serosa (visceral peritoneum) with metastasis in 3–6 regional lymph nodes; or tumor penetrates subserosal connective tissue without invasion of visceral peritoneum or adjacent structures with metastasis in 7 or more regional lymph nodes
IIIC
Tumor invades adjacent structures with metastasis in 3 or more regional lymph nodes; or tumor invades serosa (visceral peritoneum) with metastasis in 7 or more regional lymph nodes
IV
Distant metastasis

Tuesday, December 25, 2018

CCH_Medicine is a science of uncertainty and an art of probability”

“Medicine is a science of uncertainty and an art of probability”
Chapter 3: Decision-Making in Clinical Medicine
Daniel B. Mark; John B. Wong

BRIEF INTRODUCTION TO CLINICAL REASONING
Clinical Expertise
Intuitive Versus Analytic Reasoning
DIAGNOSTIC VERSUS THERAPEUTIC DECISION-MAKING
NON-CLINICAL INFLUENCES ON CLINICAL DECISION-MAKING
Factors Related to Practice Style
Practice Setting Factors
Payment Systems
INTERPRETATION OF DIAGNOSTIC TESTS
DIAGNOSTIC TESTING: MEASURES OF TEST ACCURACY
MEASURES OF DISEASE PROBABILITY AND BAYES’ RULE
APPLICATIONS TO DIAGNOSTIC TESTING IN CAD
STATISTICAL PREDICTION MODELS
FORMAL DECISION SUPPORT TOOLS
DECISION SUPPORT SYSTEMS
DECISION ANALYSIS
DIAGNOSIS AS AN ELEMENT OF QUALITY OF CARE
EVIDENCE-BASED MEDICINE
SOURCES OF EVIDENCE: CLINICAL TRIALS AND REGISTRIES
META-ANALYSIS
CLINICAL PRACTICE GUIDELINES

INTRODUCTION
Sir William Osler’s familiar quote “Medicine is a science of uncertainty and an art of probability” captures well the complex nature of clinical medicine. Although the science of medicine is often taught as if the mechanisms of the human body operate with Newtonian predictability, every aspect of medical practice is infused with an element of irreducible uncertainty that the clinician ignores at her peril. Clinical medicine has deep roots in science, but it is an imprecise science. More than 100 years after the practice of medicine took its modern form, it remains at its core a craft, to which individual doctors bring varying levels of skill and understanding. With the exponential growth in medical literature and other technical information and an ever increasing number of testing and treatment options, twenty-first century physicians who seek excellence in their craft must master a more diverse and complex set of skills than any of the generations that preceded them. This chapter provides an introduction to three of the pillars upon which the craft of modern medicine rests: (1) expertise in clinical reasoning (what it is and how it can be developed); (2) rational diagnostic tests, use and interpretation; and (3) integration of the best available research evidence with clinical judgment in the care of individual patients (evidence-based medicine or EBM and the tools of EBM).

BRIEF INTRODUCTION TO CLINICAL REASONING
Clinical Expertise
Defining “clinical expertise” remains surprisingly difficult. Chess has an objective ranking system based on skill and performance criteria. Athletics, similarly, have ranking systems to distinguish novices from Olympians. But in medicine, after physicians complete training and pass the boards (or get recertified), no tests or benchmarks are used to identify those who have attained the highest levels of clinical performance. Physicians often consult a few “elite” clinicians for their “special problem-solving prowess” when particularly difficult or obscure cases have baffled everyone else. Yet despite their skill, even such master clinicians typically cannot explain their exact processes and methods, thereby limiting the acquisition and dissemination of the expertise used to achieve their impressive results. Furthermore, clinical virtuosity appears not to be generalizable, e.g., an expert on hypertrophic cardiomyopathy may be no better (and possibly worse) than a first-year medical resident at diagnosing and managing a patient with neutropenia, fever, and hypotension.

Broadly construed, clinical expertise includes not only cognitive dimensions involving the integration of disease knowledge with verbal and visual cues and test interpretation but also potentially the complex fine-motor skills necessary for invasive procedures and tests. In addition, “the complete package” of expertise in medicine requires effective communication and care coordination with patients and members of the medical team. Research on medical expertise remains sparse overall and mostly centered on diagnostic reasoning, so in this chapter, we focus primarily on the cognitive elements of clinical reasoning.

Because clinical reasoning occurs in the heads of clinicians, objective study of the process is difficult. One research method used for this area asks clinicians to “think out loud” as they receive increments of clinical information in a manner meant to simulate a clinical encounter. Another research approach focuses on how doctors should reason diagnostically to identify remediable “errors” rather than on how they actually do reason. Much of what is known about clinical reasoning comes from empirical studies of nonmedical problem-solving behavior. Because of the diverse perspectives contributing to this area, with important contributions from cognitive psychology, medical education, behavioral economics, sociology, informatics, and decision sciences, no single integrated model of clinical reasoning exists, and not infrequently, different terms and reasoning models describe similar phenomena.

Intuitive Versus Analytic Reasoning
A useful contemporary model of reasoning, dual-process theory distinguishes two general systems of cognitive processes. Intuition (System 1) provides rapid effortless judgments from memorized associations using pattern recognition and other simplifying “rules of thumb” (i.e., heuristics). For example, a very simple pattern that could be useful in certain situations is “African-American women plus hilar adenopathy equals sarcoid.” Because no effort is involved in recalling the pattern, typically, the clinician is unable to say how those judgments were formulated. In contrast, Analysis (System 2), the other form of reasoning in the dual-process model, is slow, methodical, deliberative, and effortful. A student might read about lymph nodes in the lung and from that list (e.g., Chap. 62), identify diseases more common in African-American women or examine the patient for skin or eye findings that may occur with sarcoid. These dual processes, of course, represent two exemplars taken from the cognitive continuum. They provide helpful descriptive insights but very little guidance in how to develop expertise in clinical reasoning. How these idealized systems interact in different decision problems, how experts use them differently from novices, and when their use can lead to errors in judgment remain the subject of study and considerable debate.

Pattern recognition, an important part of System 1 reasoning, is a complex cognitive process that appears largely effortless. One can recognize people’s faces, the breed of a dog, an automobile model, or a piece of music from just a few notes within milliseconds without necessarily being able to articulate the specific features that prompted the recognition. Analogously, experienced clinicians often recognize familiar diagnosis patterns very quickly. The key here is having a large library of stored patterns that can be rapidly accessed. In the absence of an extensive stored repertoire of diagnostic patterns, students (as well as more experienced clinicians operating outside their area of expertise and familiarity) often must use the more laborious System 2 analytic approach along with more intensive and comprehensive data collection to reach the diagnosis.

The following three brief scenarios of a patient with hemoptysis illustrate three distinct patterns that experienced clinicians recognize without effort:

A 46-year-old man presents to his internist with a chief complaint of hemoptysis. An otherwise healthy, nonsmoker, he is recovering from an apparent viral bronchitis. This presentation pattern suggests that the small amount of blood-streaked sputum is due to acute bronchitis, so that a chest x-ray provides sufficient reassurance that a more serious disorder is absent.

In the second scenario, a 46-year-old patient who has the same chief complaint but with a 100-pack-year smoking history, a productive morning cough, with blood-streaked sputum, and weight loss fits the pattern of carcinoma of the lung. Consequently, along with the chest x-ray, the clinician obtains a sputum cytology examination and refers this patient for a chest CT scan.

In the third scenario, the clinician hears a soft diastolic rumbling murmur at the apex on cardiac auscultation in a 46-year-old patient with hemoptysis who immigrated from a developing country and orders an echocardiogram as well, because of possible pulmonary hypertension from suspected rheumatic mitral stenosis.

Pattern recognition by itself is not, however, sufficient for secure diagnosis. Without deliberative systematic reflection, pattern recognition can result in premature closure: mistakenly jumping to the conclusion that one has correct diagnosis before all the relevant data are in. A critical second step, even when the diagnosis seems obvious, is diagnostic verification: considering whether the diagnosis adequately accounts for the presenting symptoms and signs and can explain all the ancillary findings. An example of premature closure is contained in the following case, modified from a real clinical encounter. A 45-year-old man presents with a 3-week history of a “flulike” upper respiratory infection (URI) including dyspnea and a productive cough. The Emergency Department (ED) clinician pulled out a “URI assessment form” which defines and standardizes the information gathered. After quickly acquiring the requisite structured examination components and noting in particular the absence of fever and a clear chest examination, the physician prescribed a cough suppressant for acute bronchitis and reassured the patient that his illness was not serious. Following a sleepless night at home with significant dyspnea, the patient developed nausea and vomiting and collapsed. He was brought back to the ED in cardiac arrest and was unable to be resuscitated. His autopsy showed a posterior wall myocardial infarction (MI) and a fresh thrombus in an atherosclerotic right coronary artery. What went wrong? Presumably, the ED clinician felt that the patient was basically healthy (one can be misled by the way the patient appears on examination—a patient that does not “appear sick” may be incorrectly assumed to have an innocuous illness). So in this case, the physician, upon hearing the overview of the patient from the triage nurse, elected to use the URI assessment protocol even before starting the history, closing consideration of the broader range of possibilities and associated tests required to confirm or refute these possibilities. In particular, by concentrating on the abbreviated and focused URI protocol, the clinician failed to elicit the full dyspnea history, which was precipitated by exertion and accompanied by chest heaviness and relieved by rest, suggesting a far more serious disorder.

Heuristics or rules of thumb are a part of the intuitive system. These cognitive shortcuts provide a quick and easy path to reaching conclusions and making choices, but when used improperly they can lead to errors. Two major research programs have studied heuristics in a mostly non-medical context and have reached very different conclusions about the value of these cognitive tools. The “heuristics and biases” program focuses on how relying on heuristics can lead to cognitive biases and incorrect judgments. Over 100 different cognitive biases have been described. So far, however, there is little evidence that educating physicians and other decision makers to watch for these cognitive biases has any effect on the rate of diagnostic errors. In contrast, the “fast and frugal heuristics” research program explores how and when relying on simple heuristics can produce good decisions. Although many heuristics have relevance to clinical reasoning, only four will be mentioned here.

When diagnosing patients, clinicians usually develop diagnostic hypotheses based on the similarity of that patient’s symptoms, signs and other data to their mental representations (memorized patterns) of the disease possibilities. In other words, clinicians pattern match to identify the diagnoses which share the most similar findings to the patient at hand. This cognitive shortcut is called the representativeness heuristic. Consider a patient with hypertension and headache, palpitations, and diaphoresis. Based on the representativeness heuristic, clinicians might judge pheochromocytoma to be quite likely given this classic presenting symptom triad suggesting pheochromocytoma. Doing so however, would be incorrect given that other causes of hypertension are much more common than pheochromocytoma and this triad of symptoms can occur in patients who do not have it. Thus, clinicians using the representativeness heuristic may overestimate the likelihood of a particular disease based on its representativeness by failing to recognize the low underlying prevalence (i.e., the prior, or pretest, probabilities). Conversely, atypical presentations of common diseases may lead to underestimating the likelihood of a particular disease. Thus, inexperience with a specific disease and with the breadth of its presentations may also lead to diagnostic delays or errors, e.g., diseases that affect multiple organ systems, such as sarcoid or tuberculosis, may be particularly challenging to diagnose because of the many different patterns they may manifest.

A second commonly used cognitive shortcut, the availability heuristic, involves judgments based on how easily prior similar cases or outcomes can be brought to mind. For example, a clinician may recall a case from a morbidity and mortality conference in which an elderly patient presented with painless dyspnea of acute onset and was evaluated for a pulmonary cause, but eventually found to have acute MI with the diagnostic delay likely contributing to the development of ischemic cardiomyopathy. If the case was associated with a malpractice accusation, such examples may be even more memorable. Errors with the availability heuristic arise from several sources of recall bias. Rare catastrophes are likely to be remembered with a clarity and force disproportionate to their likelihood for future diagnosis—for example, a patient with a sore throat eventually found to have leukemia or a young athlete with leg pain subsequently found to have a sarcoma—and those publicized in the media or recent experience are, of course, easier to recall and therefore more influential on clinical judgments.

The third commonly used cognitive shortcut, the anchoring heuristic (also called conservatism or stickiness), involves insufficiently adjusting the initial probability of disease up (or down) following a positive (or negative test) when compared with Bayes’ theorem, i.e., sticking to the initial diagnosis. For example, a clinician may still judge the probability of coronary artery disease (CAD) to be high despite a negative exercise perfusion test and go on to cardiac catheterization (see “Measures of Disease Probability and Bayes’ Rule,” below).

The fourth heuristic states that clinicians should use the simplest explanation possible that will adequately account for the patient’s symptoms and findings (Occam’s razor or alternatively the simplicity heuristic). Although this is an attractive and often used principle, it is important to remember that no biologic basis for it exists. Errors from the simplicity heuristic include premature closure leading to the neglect of unexplained significant symptoms or findings.

For complex or unfamiliar diagnostic problems, clinicians typically resort to analytic reasoning processes (System 2) and proceed methodically using the hypothetico-deductive model of reasoning. Based on the stated reasons for seeking medical attention, clinicians develop an initial list of diagnostic possibilities in hypothesis generation. During the history of the present illness, the initial hypotheses evolve in diagnostic refinement as emerging information is tested against the mental models of the diseases being considered with diagnoses increasing and decreasing in likelihood or even being dropped from consideration as the working hypotheses of the moment. These mental models often generate additional questions that distinguish the diagnostic possibilities from one another. The focused physical examination contributes further distinguishing the working hypotheses. Is the spleen enlarged? How big is the liver? Is it tender? Are there any palpable masses or nodules? Diagnostic verification involves testing the adequacy (whether the diagnosis accounts for all symptoms and signs) and coherency (whether the signs and symptoms are consistent with the underlying pathophysiological causal mechanism) of the diagnosis. For example, if the enlarged and quite tender liver felt on physical examination is due to acute hepatitis (the hypothesis), then certain specific liver function tests will be markedly elevated (the prediction). Should the tests come back normal, the hypothesis may have to be discarded or substantially modified.

Although often neglected, negative findings are as important as positive ones because they reduce the likelihood of the diagnostic hypotheses under consideration. Chest discomfort that is not provoked or worsened by exertion and not relieved by rest in an active patient reduces the likelihood that chronic ischemic heart disease is the underlying cause. The absence of a resting tachycardia and thyroid gland enlargement reduces the likelihood of hyperthyroidism in a patient with paroxysmal atrial fibrillation.

The acuity of a patient’s illness may override considerations of prevalence and the other issues described above. “Diagnostic imperatives” recognize the significance of relatively rare but potentially catastrophic diagnoses if undiagnosed and untreated. For example, clinicians should consider aortic dissection routinely as a possible cause of acute severe chest discomfort. Although the typical presenting symptoms of dissection differ from that of MI, dissection may mimic MI, and because it is far less prevalent and potentially fatal if mistreated, diagnosing dissection remains a challenging diagnostic imperative (Chap. 274). Clinicians taking care of acute, severe chest pain patients should explicitly and routinely inquire about symptoms suggestive of dissection, measure blood pressures in both arms for discrepancies, and examine for pulse deficits. When these are all negative, clinicians may feel sufficiently reassured to discard the aortic dissection hypothesis. If, however, the chest x-ray shows a possible widened mediastinum, the hypothesis should be reinstated and an appropriate imaging test ordered (e.g., thoracic computed tomography [CT] scan or transesophageal echocardiogram). In non-acute situations, the prevalence of potential alternative diagnoses should play a much more prominent role in diagnostic hypothesis generation.

Cognitive scientists studying the thought processes of expert clinicians have observed that clinicians group data into packets, or “chunks,” that are stored in short-term or “working memory” and manipulated to generate diagnostic hypotheses. Because short-term memory is limited (classically humans can accurately repeat a list of 7±2 numbers read to them), the number of diagnoses that can be actively considered in hypothesis-generating activities is similarly limited. For this reason, cognitive shortcuts discussed above play a key role in the generation of diagnostic hypotheses, many of which are discarded as rapidly as they are formed, thereby demonstrating that the distinction between analytic and intuitive reasoning is an arbitrary and simplistic, but nonetheless useful, representation of cognition.

Research into the hypothetico-deductive model of reasoning has had difficulty identifying the elements of the reasoning process that distinguish experts from novices. This has led to a shift from examining the problem-solving process of experts to analyzing the organization of their knowledge for pattern matching as exemplars, prototypes, and illness scripts. For example, diagnosis may be based on the resemblance of a new case to patients seen previously (exemplars). As abstract mental models of disease, prototypes incorporate the likelihood of various disease features. Illness scripts include risk factors, pathophysiology, and symptoms and signs. Experts have a much larger store of exemplar and prototype cases, an example of which is the visual long-term memory of experienced radiologists. However, clinicians do not simply rely on literal recall of specific cases but have constructed elaborate conceptual networks of memorized information or models of disease to aid in arriving at their conclusions (illness scripts). That is, expertise involves an enhanced ability to connect symptoms, signs, and risk factors to one another in meaningful ways; relate those findings to possible diagnoses; and identify the additional information necessary to confirm the diagnosis.

No single theory accounts for all the key features of expertise in medical diagnosis. Experts have more knowledge about presenting symptoms of diseases and a larger repertoire of cognitive tools to employ in problem solving than non-experts. One definition of expertise highlights the ability to make powerful distinctions. In this sense, expertise involves a working knowledge of the diagnostic possibilities and those features that distinguish one disease from another. Memorization alone is insufficient, e.g., photographic memory of a medical textbook would not make one an expert. But having access to detailed case-specific relevant information is critically important. In the past, clinicians primarily acquired clinical knowledge through their patient experiences, but now clinicians have access to a plethora of information sources (see Evidence-Based Medicine [EBM] below). Clinicians of the future will be able to leverage the experiences of large numbers of other clinicians using electronic tools, but, as with the memorized textbook, the data alone will be insufficient for becoming an expert. Nonetheless, availability of these data removes one barrier for acquiring experience with connecting symptoms, signs, and risk factors to the possible diagnoses and identifying the additional distinguishing information necessary to confirm the diagnosis, thereby potentially facilitating the development of the working knowledge necessary for becoming an expert.

Despite all of the research seeking to understand expertise in medicine and other disciplines, it remains uncertain whether any didactic program can actually accelerate the progression from novice to expert or from experienced clinician to master clinician. Deliberate effortful practice (over an extended period of time, sometimes said to be 10 years or 10,000 practice hours) and personal coaching are two strategies that are often used outside medicine (e.g., music, athletics, chess) to promote expertise. Their use in developing medical expertise and maintaining or enhancing it has not yet been adequately explored. Some studies in medicine suggest that didactic education exposing students to both the signs and symptoms of specific diseases and, in addition, the diseases that may present with specific symptoms and signs may be beneficial. Developing a personal learning system (e.g., metacognition) through for example EBM processes below and follow-up to identify diagnoses and treatments for patients that you have cared for provide active learning opportunities.

DIAGNOSTIC VERSUS THERAPEUTIC DECISION-MAKING
The modern ideal of medical therapeutic decision making is to “personalize” treatment recommendations. In the abstract, personalizing treatment involves combining the best available evidence about what works with an individual patient’s unique features (e.g., risk factors, genomics and co-morbidities) and his or her preferences and health goals to craft an optimal treatment recommendation with the patient. Operationally, two different and complementary levels of personalization are possible: individualizing the risk of harm and benefit for the options being considered based on the specific patient characteristics (precision medicine), and personalizing the therapeutic decision process by incorporating the patient’s preferences and values for the possible health outcomes. This latter process is sometimes referred to as shared decision-making, and typically involves clinicians sharing their knowledge about the options and the associated consequences and tradeoffs, and patients sharing their health goals, e.g., avoiding a short-term risk of dying from coronary artery bypass grafting to see their grandchild get married in a few months.

Individualizing the evidence about therapy does not mean relying on physician impressions of benefit and harm from their personal experience. Because of small sample sizes and rare events, the chance of drawing erroneous causal inferences from one’s own clinical experience is very high. For most chronic diseases, therapeutic effectiveness is only demonstrable statistically in large patient populations. It would be incorrect to infer with any certainty, for example, that treating a hypertensive patient with angiotensin-converting enzyme (ACE) inhibitors necessarily prevented a stroke from occurring during treatment, or that an untreated patient would definitely have avoided their stroke had they been treated. For many chronic diseases, a majority of patients will remain event free regardless of treatment choices; some will have events regardless of which treatment is selected; and those who avoided having an event through treatment cannot be individually identified. Blood pressure lowering, a readily observable surrogate endpoint, does not have a tightly coupled relationship with strokes prevented. Consequently, in most situations demonstrating therapeutic effectiveness cannot rely simply on observing the outcome of an individual patient but should instead be based on large groups of patients carefully studied and properly analyzed.

Therapeutic decision-making, therefore, should be based on the best available evidence from clinical trials and well done outcome studies. Trustworthy clinical practice guidelines that synthesize such evidence offer normative guidance for many testing and treatment decisions. However, all guidelines recognize that “one size fits all” recommendations may not apply to individual patients. Increased research into the heterogeneity of treatment effects seeks to understand how best to adjust group level clinical evidence of treatment harms and benefits to account for the absolute level of risks faced by subgroups and even by individual patients, using, for example, validated clinical risk scores.

NON-CLINICAL INFLUENCES ON CLINICAL DECISION-MAKING
More than three decades of research on variations in clinician practice patterns has identified important non-clinical forces that shape clinical decisions. These factors can be grouped conceptually into three overlapping categories: (1) factors related to individual physicians practice, (2) factors related to practice setting, and (3) factors related to payment systems.

Factors Related to Practice Style
To ensure that necessary care is provided at a high level of quality, physicians fulfill a key role in medical care by serving as the patient’s advocate. Factors that influence performance in this role include the physician’s knowledge, training, and experience. Clearly, physicians cannot practice evidence-based medicine if they are unfamiliar with the evidence. As would be expected, specialists generally know the evidence in their field better than do generalists. Beyond published evidence and practice guidelines, a major set of influences on physician practice can be subsumed under the general concept of “practice style.” The practice style serves to define norms of clinical behavior. Beliefs about effectiveness of different therapies and preferred patterns of diagnostic test use are examples of different facets of a practice style. The physician beliefs that drive these different practice styles may be based on training, personal experience, and medical evidence. For example, in heart failure patients, heart failure specialists have more familiarity than general internists with the target doses of ACE inhibitor therapy as defined by large clinical trials and the specific drugs (including adverse effects), and are less likely to overreact to foreseeable problems in therapy such as a rise in creatinine levels or asymptomatic hypotension. Not surprisingly, the specialists are much more likely than generalists to achieve target doses of ACE inhibitor therapy. By contrast, perhaps due to specialization, cardiologists may overestimate the benefit and underestimate the harm of coronary revascularization relative to general internists.

Beyond the patient’s welfare, physician perceptions about the risk of a malpractice suit resulting from either an erroneous decision or a bad outcome may drive clinical decisions and create a practice referred to as defensive medicine. This practice involves using tests and therapies with very small marginal benefits, ostensibly to preclude future criticism should an adverse outcome occur. With conscious or unconscious awareness of a connection to the risk of litigation or to payment, however, over time such patterns of care may become accepted as part of the practice norm, thereby perpetuating their overuse, e.g., annual cardiac exercise testing in asymptomatic patients.

Practice Setting Factors
Factors in this category relate to work systems including tasks and workflow (interruptions, inefficiencies, workload), technology (poor design or implementation, errors in use, failure, misuse), organizational characteristics (e.g., culture, leadership, staffing, scheduling), and the physical environment (e.g., noise, lighting, layout). Physician-induced demand is a term that refers to the repeated observation that once medical facilities and technologies become available to physicians, they will use them. Other environmental factors that can influence decision-making include the local availability of specialists for consultations and procedures; “high-tech” advanced imaging or procedure facilities such as MRI machines and proton beam therapy centers; and fragmentation of care.

Payment Systems
Economic incentives are closely related to the other two categories of practice-modifying factors. Financial issues can exert both stimulatory and inhibitory influences on clinical practice. Historically, physicians are paid on a fee-for-service, capitation, or salary basis. In fee-for-service, physicians who do more get paid more, thereby encouraging overuse, consciously or unconsciously. When fees are reduced (discounted reimbursement), clinicians tend to increase the number of services provided to maintain revenue. Capitation, in contrast, provides a fixed payment per patient per year to encourage physicians to consider a global population budget in managing individual patients and ideally reducing the use of interventions with small marginal benefit. To discourage volume-based excessive utilization, fixed salary compensation plans pay physicians the same regardless of the clinical effort expended, but may provide an incentive to see fewer patients. In recognition of the non-sustainability of continued growth in medical expenditures and the opportunity costs associated with that (funds that might be more beneficially applied to education, energy, social welfare or defense), current efforts seek to transition to a value-based payment system to reduce overuse and to reflect benefit. Work to define how to actually tie payment to value has mostly focused so far on “pay for performance” models. High quality clinical trial evidence for the effectiveness of these models is still mostly lacking.

INTERPRETATION OF DIAGNOSTIC TESTS
DIAGNOSTIC TESTING: MEASURES OF TEST ACCURACY
MEASURES OF DISEASE PROBABILITY AND BAYES’ RULE
APPLICATIONS TO DIAGNOSTIC TESTING IN CAD
Consider two tests commonly used in the diagnosis of CAD: an exercise treadmill and an exercise single-photon emission CT (SPECT) myocardial perfusion imaging test (Chap. 236). Meta-analysis has shown that a positive treadmill ST-segment response has an average sensitivity of 60% and an average specificity of 75%, yielding a likelihood ratio positive of 2.4 (0.60/[1 – 0.75]) (consistent with modest discriminatory ability because it falls between 2 and 5). For a 41-year-old man with nonanginal pain and a 10% pretest probability of CAD, the posttest probability of disease after a positive result rises to only about 30%. For a 60-year-old woman with typical angina and a pretest probability of CAD of 80%, a positive test result raises the posttest probability of disease to about 95%.

In contrast, exercise SPECT myocardial perfusion test is more accurate for diagnosis of CAD. For simplicity, assume that the finding of a reversible exercise-induced perfusion defect has both a sensitivity and a specificity of 90% (a bit higher than reported), yielding a likelihood ratio for a positive test of 9.0 (0.90/[1 – 0.90]) (consistent with intermediate discriminatory ability because it falls between 5 and 10). For the same 10% pretest probability patient, a positive test raises the probability of CAD to 50% (Fig. 3-2). However, despite the differences in posttest probabilities between these two tests (30 versus 50%), the more accurate test may not improve diagnostic likelihood enough to change patient management (e.g., decision to refer to cardiac catheterization) because the more accurate test has only moved the physician from being fairly certain that the patient did not have CAD to a 50:50 chance of disease. In a patient with a pretest probability of 80%, exercise SPECT test raises the posttest probability to 97% (compared with 95% for the exercise treadmill). Again, the more accurate test does not provide enough improvement in posttest confidence to alter management, and neither test has improved much on what was known from clinical data alone.

In general, positive results with an accurate test (e.g., likelihood ratio positive 10) when the pretest probability is low (e.g., 20%) do not move the posttest probability to a range high enough to rule in disease (e.g., 80%). In screening situations, pretest probabilities are often particularly low because patients are asymptomatic. In such cases, specificity becomes particularly important. For example, in screening first-time female blood donors without risk factors for HIV, a positive test raised the likelihood of HIV to only 67% despite a specificity of 99.995% because the prevalence was 0.01%. Conversely, with a high pretest probability, a negative test may not rule out disease adequately if it is not sufficiently sensitive. Thus, the largest change in diagnostic likelihood following a test result occurs when the clinician is most uncertain (i.e., pretest probability between 30 and 70%). For example, if a patient has a pretest probability for CAD of 50%, a positive exercise treadmill test will move the posttest probability to 80% and a positive exercise SPECT perfusion test will move it to 90% (Fig. 3-2).

As presented above, Bayes’ rule employs a number of important simplifications that should be considered. First, few tests provide only “positive” or “negative” results. Many tests have multi-dimensional outcomes (e.g., extent of ST-segment depression, exercise duration, and exercise-induced symptoms with exercise testing). Although Bayes’ theorem can be adapted to this more detailed test result format, it is computationally more complex to do so. Similarly, when multiple sequential tests are performed, the posttest probability may be used as the pretest probability to interpret the second test. However, this simplification assumes conditional independence—that is, that the results of the first test do not affect the likelihood of the second test result—and this is often not true.

Finally, many texts assert that sensitivity and specificity are prevalence-independent parameters of test accuracy. This statistically useful assumption, however, is clinically simplistic. A treadmill exercise test, for example, has a sensitivity of ~30% in a population of patients with 1-vessel CAD, whereas its sensitivity in patients with severe 3-vessel CAD approaches 80%. Thus, the best estimate of sensitivity to use in a particular decision may vary, depending on the severity of disease in the local population. A hospitalized, symptomatic, or referral population typically has a higher prevalence of disease and, in particular, a higher prevalence of more advanced disease than does an outpatient population. Consequently, test sensitivity will likely be higher in hospitalized patients, and test specificity higher in outpatients.

STATISTICAL PREDICTION MODELS
Bayes’ rule, when used as presented above, is useful in studying diagnostic testing concepts but may prove too simplistic for use in actual patient care decisions. Predictions based on multivariable statistical models can more accurately address these more complex problems by simultaneously accounting for additional relevant patient characteristics. In particular, these models explicitly account for multiple, even possibly overlapping, pieces of patient-specific information and assign a relative weight to each on the basis of its unique independent contribution to the prediction in question. For example, a logistic regression model to predict the probability of CAD ideally considers all the relevant independent factors from the clinical examination and diagnostic testing and their relative importance instead of the limited data that clinicians can manage in their heads or with Bayes’ rule. However, despite this strength, prediction models are usually too complex computationally to use without a calculator or computer. Guideline-driven treatment recommendations based on statistical prediction models available online, e.g., the ACC/AHA risk calculator for primary prevention with statins and the CHA2DS2-VASC calculator for anticoagulation for atrial fibrillation have generated more widespread usage. Whether the adoption of electronic health records will promote more use of predictive models in clinical practice and increase their impact on clinical encounters and outcomes remains unclear.

One reason for limited clinical use is that, to date, only a handful of prediction models have been validated properly (for example, Wells’ criteria for pulmonary embolism, see Table 3-2). The importance of independent validation in a population separate from the one used to develop the model cannot be overstated. An unvalidated prediction model should be viewed with the skepticism appropriate for any new drug or medical device that has not had rigorous clinical trial testing.

TABLE 3-2
Wells Clinical Prediction Rule for Pulmonary Embolism
View Table|  Favorite Table|Download (.pdf)
When statistical survival models in cancer and heart disease have been compared directly with clinicians’ predictions, the survival models have been found to be more consistent, as would be expected but not always more accurate. On the other hand, comparison of clinicians with websites and apps that generate lists of possible diagnoses to help patients with self-diagnosis found that physicians outperformed the currently available programs. For students and less-experienced clinicians, the biggest value of diagnostic decision support may be in extending diagnostic possibilities and triggering “rational override” but their impact on knowledge, information-seeking, and problem-solving needs additional research.

FORMAL DECISION SUPPORT TOOLS
DECISION SUPPORT SYSTEMS
Over the last 40 years, many attempts have been made to develop computer systems to aid clinical decision-making and patient management. Conceptually, computers offer several levels of potentially useful support for clinicians. At the most basic level, they provide ready access to vast reservoirs of information, which may, however, be quite difficult to sort through to find what is needed. At higher levels, computers can support care management decisions by making accurate predictions of outcome, or can simulate the whole decision process, and provide algorithmic guidance. Computer-based predictions using Bayesian or statistical regression models inform a clinical decision but do not actually reach a “conclusion” or “recommendation.” Machine learning methods are being applied to pattern recognition tasks such as the examination of skin lesions and the interpretation of x-rays. Artificial intelligence systems attempt to simulate or replace human reasoning with a computer-based analogue. To date, such approaches have achieved only limited success. Reminder or protocol-directed systems do not make predictions but use existing algorithms, such as guidelines or appropriate utilization criteria, to direct clinical practice. In general, however, decision support systems have had little impact on practice. Reminder systems built into electronic health records have shown the most promise, particularly in correcting drug dosing and promoting adherence to guidelines. Checklists may also help avoid or reduce errors.

DECISION ANALYSIS
Compared with the decision support methods above, decision analysis represents a normative prescriptive approach to decision-making in the face of uncertainty. Its principal application is in complex decisions. For example, public health policy decisions often involve trade-offs in length versus quality of life, benefits versus resource use, population versus individual health, and uncertainty regarding efficacy, effectiveness, and adverse events as well as values or preferences regarding mortality and morbidity outcomes.

One recent analysis using this approach involved the optimal screening strategy for breast cancer, which has remained controversial, in part because a randomized controlled trial to determine when to begin screening and how often to repeat screening mammography is impractical. In 2016, the National Cancer Institute sponsored Cancer Intervention and Surveillance Network (CISNET) examined eight strategies differing by whether to initiate mammography screening at age 40, 45, or 50 years and whether to screen annually, biennially, or annually for women in their forties and biennially thereafter (hybrid). The six simulation models found biennial strategies to be the most efficient for average-risk women. Biennial screening for 1000 women from age 50 to 74 years versus no screening avoided seven breast cancer deaths. Screening annually from age 40 to 74 years avoided three additional deaths but required 20,000 additional mammograms and yielded 1988 more false-positive results. Factors that influenced the results included patients with a 2–4-fold higher risk for developing breast cancer in whom annual screening from 40 to 74 yielded similar benefits as biennial screening from age 50 to 74. For average-risk patients with moderate or severe co-morbidities, screening could be stopped earlier at ages 66–68 years.

This analysis involved six models that reproduced epidemiologic trends and a screening trial result, accounted for digital technology and treatments advances, and considered quality of life, risk factors, breast density, and comorbidity. It provided novel insights into a public health problem in the absence of a randomized clinical trial and helped weigh the pros and cons of such a health policy recommendation. Although such models have been developed for selected clinical problems, their benefit and application to individual real-time clinical management has yet to be demonstrated.

DIAGNOSIS AS AN ELEMENT OF QUALITY OF CARE
High quality medical care begins with accurate diagnosis. The incidence of diagnostic errors has been estimated by a variety of methods including postmortem examinations, medical record reviews, and medical malpractice claims, with each yielding complementary but different estimates of this quality of care patient-safety problem. In the past, diagnostic errors tended to be viewed as a failure of individual clinicians. The modern view is that they are mostly system of care deficiencies. Current estimates suggest that nearly everyone will experience at least one diagnostic error in their lifetime, leading to mortality, morbidity, unnecessary tests and procedures, costs, and anxiety.

Solutions to the “diagnostic errors as a system of care problem” have focused on system-level approaches, such as decision support and other tools integrated into electronic medical records. The use of checklists has been proposed as a means of reducing some of the cognitive errors discussed earlier in the chapter, such as premature closure. While checklists have been shown useful in certain medical contexts, such as the ORs and ICUs, their value in preventing diagnostic errors that lead to patient adverse events remains to be shown.

EVIDENCE-BASED MEDICINE
Clinical medicine is defined traditionally as a practice combining medical knowledge (including scientific evidence), intuition, and judgment in the care of patients (Chap. 1). Evidence-based medicine (EBM) updates this construct by placing much greater emphasis on the processes by which clinicians gain knowledge of the most up-to-date and relevant clinical research to determine for themselves whether medical interventions alter the disease course and improve the length or quality of life. The meaning of practicing EBM becomes clearer through an examination of its four key steps:

Formulating the management question to be answered

Searching the literature and online databases for applicable research data

Appraising the evidence gathered with regard to its validity and relevance

Integrating this appraisal with knowledge about the unique aspects of the patient (including the patient’s preferences about the possible outcomes)

The process of searching the world’s research literature and appraising the quality and relevance of studies can be time-consuming and requires skills and training that most clinicians do not possess. Thus, identifying recent systematic overviews of the problem in question (Table 3-3) may offer the best starting point for most EBM searches. However, the medical literature is now being flooded with systematic reviews of varying quality and clinical utility. Therefore, systematic reviews should be used in conjunction with selective reading of some of the best empirical studies.

TABLE 3-3
Selected Tools for Finding the Evidence in Evidence-Based Medicine
View Table|  Favorite Table|Download (.pdf)
Generally, the EBM tools listed in Table 3-3 provide access to research information in one of two forms. The first, primary research reports, is the original peer-reviewed research work that is published in medical journals and accessible through MEDLINE in abstract form. However, without training in using MEDLINE, locating reports quickly and efficiently that are on point in a sea of irrelevant or unhelpful citations remains difficult, and important studies are easily missed. Systematic reviews, the second form, are regarded by some as the highest level of evidence in the hierarchy because they are intended to comprehensively summarize the available evidence on a particular topic. To avoid the potential biases found in review articles, predefined reproducible explicit search strategies and inclusion and exclusion criteria seek to find all of the relevant scientific research and grade its quality. The prototype for this kind of resource is the Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. When appropriate, a meta-analysis is used to quantitatively summarize the systematic review findings. The next two sections explicate the major types of clinical research reports available in the literature and the process of aggregating those data into meta-analyses.

SOURCES OF EVIDENCE: CLINICAL TRIALS AND REGISTRIES
The notion of learning from observation of patients is as old as medicine itself. Over the last 50 years, physicians’ understanding of how best to turn raw observation into useful evidence has evolved considerably. Case reports, personal anecdotal experience, and small single-center case series are now recognized as having severe limitations in validity and generalizability, and although they may generate hypotheses or be the first reports of adverse events or therapeutic benefit, they have no role in formulating modern standards of practice. The major tools used to develop reliable evidence consist of the randomized clinical trial and the large observational registry. A registry or database typically is focused on a disease or syndrome (e.g., different types of cancer, acute or chronic CAD, pacemaker capture or chronic heart failure), a clinical procedure (e.g., bone marrow transplantation, coronary revascularization), or an administrative process (e.g., claims data used for billing and reimbursement).

By definition, in observational data, the investigator does not control patient care. Carefully collected prospective observational data, however, can at times achieve a level of evidence quality approaching that of major clinical trial data. At the other end of the spectrum, data collected retrospectively (e.g., chart review) are limited in form and content to what previous observers recorded and may not include the specific research data being sought (e.g., claims data). Advantages of observational data include the inclusion of a broader population as encountered in practice than is typically represented in clinical trials because of their restrictive inclusion and exclusion criteria. In addition, observational data provide primary evidence for research questions when a randomized trial cannot be performed. For example, it would be difficult to randomize patients to test diagnostic or therapeutic strategies that are unproven but widely accepted in practice, and it would be unethical to randomize based on sex, racial/ethnic group, socioeconomic status, or country of residence or to randomize patients to a potentially harmful intervention, such as smoking or deliberately overeating to develop obesity.

A well-done prospective observational study of a particular management strategy differs from a well-done randomized clinical trial most importantly by its lack of protection from treatment selection bias. The use of observational data to compare diagnostic or therapeutic strategies assumes that sufficient uncertainty and heterogeneity exists in clinical practice to ensure that similar patients will be managed differently by diverse physicians. In short, the analysis assumes that a sufficient element of randomness (in the sense of disorder rather than in the formal statistical sense) exists in clinical management. In such cases, statistical models attempt to adjust for important imbalances to “level the playing field” so that a fair comparison among treatment options can be made. When management is clearly not random (e.g., all eligible left main CAD patients are referred for coronary bypass surgery), the problem may be too confounded (biased) for statistical correction, and observational data may not provide reliable evidence.

In general, the use of concurrent controls is vastly preferable to that of historical controls. For example, comparison of current surgical management of left main CAD with medically treated patients with left main CAD during the 1970s (the last time these patients were routinely treated with medicine alone) would be extremely misleading because “medical therapy” has substantially improved in the interim.

Randomized controlled clinical trials include the careful prospective design features of the best observational data studies but also include the use of random allocation of treatment. This design provides the best protection against measured and unmeasured confounding due to treatment selection bias (a major aspect of internal validity). However, the randomized trial may not have good external validity (generalizability) if the process of recruitment into the trial resulted in the exclusion of many potentially eligible subjects or if the nominal eligibility for the trial describe a very heterogeneous population.

Consumers of medical evidence need to be aware that randomized trials vary widely in their quality and applicability to practice. The process of designing such a trial often involves many compromises. For example, trials designed to gain U.S. Food and Drug Administration (FDA) approval for an investigational drug or device must fulfill regulatory requirements (such as the use of a placebo control) that may result in a trial population and design that differs substantially from what practicing clinicians would find most useful.

META-ANALYSIS
The Greek prefix meta signifies something at a later or higher stage of development. Meta-analysis is research that combines and summarizes the available evidence quantitatively. Although it is used to examine nonrandomized studies, meta-analysis is most useful for summarizing all randomized trials examining a particular therapy. Ideally, unpublished trials should be identified and included to avoid publication bias (i.e., missing “negative” trials which may not be published). Furthermore, the best meta-analyses obtain and analyze individual patient-level data from all trials rather than using only the summary data from published reports. Nonetheless, not all published meta-analyses yield reliable evidence for a particular problem, so their methodology should be scrutinized carefully to ensure proper study design and analysis. The results of a well-done meta-analysis are likely to be most persuasive if they include at least several large-scale, properly performed randomized trials. Meta-analysis can especially help detect benefits when individual trials are inadequately powered (e.g., the benefits of streptokinase thrombolytic therapy in acute MI demonstrated by ISIS-2 in 1988 were evident by the early 1970s through meta-analysis). However, in cases in which the available trials are small or poorly done, meta-analysis should not be viewed as a remedy for deficiencies in primary trial data or trial design.

Meta-analyses typically focus on summary measures of relative treatment benefit, such as odds ratios or relative risks. Clinicians also should examine what absolute risk reduction (ARR) can be expected from the therapy. A summary metric of absolute treatment benefit is the number needed to treat (NNT) to prevent one adverse outcome event (e.g., death, stroke). NNT is simply 1/ARR. For example, if a hypothetical therapy reduced mortality rates over a 5-year follow-up by 33% (the relative treatment benefit) from 12% (control arm) to 8% (treatment arm), the absolute risk reduction would be 12% – 8% = 4% and the NNT would be 1/.04, or 25. Thus, it would be necessary to treat 25 patients for 5 years to prevent 1 death. If the hypothetical treatment was applied to a lower-risk population, say, with a 6% 5-year mortality, the 33% relative treatment benefit would reduce absolute mortality by 2% (from 6 to 4%), and the NNT for the same therapy in this lower-risk group of patients would be 50. Although not always made explicit, comparisons of NNT estimates from different studies should account for the duration of follow-up used to create each estimate. In addition, the NNT concept assumes a homogeneity in response to treatment that may not be accurate. The NNT is simply another way of summarizing the absolute treatment difference and does not provide any unique information.

CLINICAL PRACTICE GUIDELINES
According to the 1990 Institute of Medicine definition, clinical practice guidelines are “systematically developed statements to assist practitioner and patient decisions about appropriate health care for specific clinical circumstances.” This definition emphasizes several crucial features of modern guideline development. First, guidelines are created by using the tools of EBM. In particular, the core of the development process is a systematic literature search followed by a review of the relevant peer-reviewed literature. Second, guidelines usually are focused on a clinical disorder (e.g., diabetes mellitus, stable angina pectoris) or a health care intervention (e.g., cancer screening). Third, the primary objective of guidelines is to improve the quality of medical care by identifying care practices which should be routinely implemented, based on high quality evidence and high benefit to harm ratios for the interventions. Guidelines are intended to “assist” decision-making, not to define explicitly what decisions should be made in a particular situation, in part because guideline level evidence alone is never sufficient for clinical decision-making (e.g., deciding whether to intubate and administer antibiotics for pneumonia in a terminally ill individual, in an individual with dementia, or in an otherwise healthy 30-year-old mother).

Guidelines are narrative documents constructed by expert panels whose composition often is determined by interested professional organizations. These panels vary in expertise and in the degree to which they represent all relevant stakeholders. The guideline documents consist of a series of specific management recommendations, a summary indication of the quantity and quality of evidence supporting each recommendation, an assessment of the benefit to harm ratio for the recommendation, and a narrative discussion of the recommendations. Many recommendations simply reflect the expert consensus of the guideline panel because literature-based evidence is insufficient or absent. The final step in guideline construction is peer review, followed by a final revision in response to the critiques provided. To improve the reliability and trustworthiness of guidelines, the National Academy of Medicine (formerly Institute of Medicine) has made methodological recommendations for guideline development.

Guidelines are closely tied to the process of quality improvement in medicine through their identification of evidence-based best practices. Such practices can be used as quality indicators. Examples include the proportion of acute MI patients who receive aspirin upon admission to a hospital and the proportion of heart failure patients with a depressed ejection fraction treated with an ACE inhibitor.

CONCLUSIONS
In this era of EBM, it is tempting to think that all the difficult decisions practitioners face have been or soon will be solved and digested into practice guidelines and computerized reminders. However, EBM provides practitioners with an ideal rather than a finished set of tools with which to manage patients. Moreover, even with such evidence, it is always worth remembering that the response to therapy of the “average” patient represented by the summary clinical trial outcomes may not be what can be expected for the specific patient sitting in front of a provider in the clinic or hospital. In addition, meta-analyses cannot generate evidence when there are no adequate randomized trials, and most of what clinicians confront in practice will never be thoroughly tested in a randomized trial. For the foreseeable future, excellent clinical reasoning skills and experience supplemented by well-designed quantitative tools and a keen appreciation for the role of individual patient preferences in their health care will continue to be of paramount importance in the practice of clinical medicine.

CCH_Chapter 2: Promoting Good Health GOALS AND APPROACHES TO PREVENTION

Chapter 2: Promoting Good Health
Donald M. Lloyd-Jones; Kathleen M. McKibbin


GOALS AND APPROACHES TO PREVENTION
HEALTH PROMOTION
PRIORITIZING PREVENTION STRATEGIES
PREVENTION AND HEALTH PROMOTION ACROSS THE LIFE COURSE
Periodic Health Evaluations
Healthy Behaviors and Lifestyles
Healthy Eating Patterns
Physical Activity
Sleep Hygiene
Weight Management
Tobacco Cessation
MENTAL HEALTH AND ADDICTION
Alcohol and Opioids
ACCIDENTS AND SUICIDE
APPROACH TO THE PATIENT

GOALS AND APPROACHES TO PREVENTION
Prevention of acute and chronic diseases before their onset has been recognized as one of the hallmarks of excellent medical practice for centuries, and is now used as a metric for highly functioning healthcare systems. The ultimate goal of preventive strategies is to avoid premature death. However, as longevity has increased dramatically worldwide over the last century (largely as a result of public health practices), increasing emphasis is placed on prevention for the purpose of preserving quality of life and extending the healthspan, not just the lifespan. Given that all patients will eventually die, the goal of prevention ultimately becomes compression of morbidity toward the end of the lifespan; that is, reduction of the amount of burden and time spent with disease prior to dying. As shown in Fig. 2-1, normative aging tends to involve a steady decline in the stock of health, with accelerating decline over time. Successful prevention offers the opportunity both to extend life and to extend healthy life, thus “squaring the curve” of health loss during aging.

FIGURE 2-1

Loss of health with aging. Representation of normative aging with loss of the full stock of health with which individuals are born (indicating gain of morbidity), contrasted with a squared curve with greater longevity and fuller stock of health (less morbidity) until shortly before death. The “squared curve” represents the likely ideal situation for most patients.

The image shows a graph depicting loss of health with aging contrasted with a squared curve with greater longevity and less morbidity by prevention.
View Full Size|  Favorite Figure|Download Slide (.ppt)
Prevention strategies have been characterized as tertiary, secondary, primary, and primordial. Tertiary prevention requires rapid action to prevent imminent death in the setting of acute illness, such as through percutaneous coronary intervention in the setting of ST-segment elevation myocardial infarction. Secondary prevention strategies focus on avoiding the recurrence of disease and death in an individual who is already affected. For example, tamoxifen is recommended for women with surgically treated early-stage, estrogen-receptor-positive breast cancer, because it reduces the risk of recurrent breast cancer (including in the contralateral breast) and death. Primary prevention attempts to reduce the risk of incident disease among individuals with a risk factor. Treatment of elevated blood pressure in individuals who have not yet experienced cardiovascular disease represents one example of primary prevention that has proven effective in reducing the incidence of stroke, heart failure, and coronary heart disease.

Primordial prevention is a more recent concept (first introduced in 1979) which focuses on prevention of the development of risk factors for disease, not just prevention of disease. Primordial prevention strategies emphasize upstream determinants of risk for chronic diseases, such as eating patterns, physical activity, and environmental and social determinants of health. It therefore encompasses medical treatment strategies for individuals as well as a strong reliance on public health and social policy. It is increasingly clear that primordial prevention represents the ultimate means for reducing the burden of chronic diseases of aging. Once risk factors develop, it is difficult to restore risk to the low level of someone who never developed the risk factor. The time spent with adverse levels of the risk factor often causes irreversible damage that precludes complete restoration of low risk. For example, individuals with hypertension who are treated back to optimal levels (<120 a="" abolish="" age="" all="" also="" and="" antihypertensive="" as="" atherosclerosis="" be="" below="" blood="" burden="" but="" cannot="" cardiovascular="" comorbid="" compared="" compression="" conversely="" damage="" described="" despite="" detail="" developing="" disease="" do="" efficacious="" elevated="" essentially="" events="" evidence="" factors="" fully="" function="" greater="" have="" having="" hypertension="" illnesses="" in="" index="" individuals="" into="" is="" left="" levels="" lifetime="" living="" longer="" lower="" maintain="" maintained="" major="" mass="" medications.="" middle="" mmhg="" morbidity="" more="" of="" optimal="" organ="" other="" p="" patients="" pressure="" prevention="" primordial="" renal="" result="" reversed="" risk="" spent="" still="" subsequently="" substantially="" such="" target="" that="" the="" their="" therapy="" they="" those="" through="" time="" treated="" twice="" untreated="" ventricular="" while="" who="" with="" without="" worse="">
Prevention strategies should be distinguished from disease screening strategies. Screening attempts to detect evidence of disease at its earliest stages, when treatment is likely to be more efficacious than for advanced disease (Chap. 4). Screening can be performed in service of prevention, especially if it aids in identifying pre-clinical markers associated with elevated disease risk.

HEALTH PROMOTION
In recent decades, medical practice has increasingly focused on public health approaches to promote health, and not just prevent disease. Prevention of disease is a worthy individual and societal goal in and of itself, but it does not necessarily guarantee health. Health is a broader construct encompassing more than just absence of disease. It includes biological, physiological, and psychological domains (among others) in a continuum, rather than occurring as a dichotomous trait. Health is therefore somewhat subjective, but attempts have been made to use more objective criteria to define health in order to raise awareness, prevent disease, and promote healthy longevity.

For example, in 2010 the American Heart Association (AHA) defined a new construct of “cardiovascular health” based on evidence of associations with longevity, disease avoidance, healthy longevity, and quality of life. The definition of cardiovascular health is based on seven health behaviors and health factors (eating pattern, physical activity, body mass, smoking status, and levels of blood pressure, blood cholesterol, and blood glucose) and includes a spectrum from poor to ideal. Individuals with optimal levels of all seven metrics simultaneously are considered to have ideal cardiovascular health. The state of cardiovascular health for an individual or a population can be assessed with simple scoring by counting the number of ideal metrics (out of 7) or applying 0 points for each poor metric, 1 point for each intermediate metric, and 2 points for each ideal metric, thus creating a composite cardiovascular health score ranging from 0 to 14 points. Higher cardiovascular health scores in younger and middle ages have been associated with greater longevity, lower incidence of cardiovascular disease, lower incidence of other chronic diseases of aging (including dementia, cancer, and more), compression of morbidity, greater quality of life, and lower healthcare costs, achieving both individual and societal goals for healthy aging, and further establishing the critical importance of primordial prevention and cardiovascular health promotion.

Focusing on health promotion, rather than just disease prevention, may also provide greater motivation for patients to pursue lifestyle changes or adhere to clinician recommendations. Extensive literature suggests that providing patients solely with information regarding disease risk, or risk reduction with treatment, is unlikely to motivate desired behavior change. Empowering patients with strategies to achieve positive health goals after discussing risks can provide more effective adherence and better long-term outcomes. In the case of smoking cessation, enumerating only the risks of smoking can lead to patient inertia and therapeutic nihilism, and has proven an ineffective approach, whereas strategies that incorporate positive health messaging, support and feedback, with appropriate use of evidence-based therapies, have proven far more effective.

PRIORITIZING PREVENTION STRATEGIES
In secondary prevention, the patient already has manifest clinical disease, and is therefore at high risk for progression. The approach should be to work with the patient to implement all evidence-based strategies that will help to prevent recurrence or progression. This will typically include drug therapy as well as therapeutic lifestyle changes to control ongoing risk factors which may have caused disease in the first place. Juggling priorities can be difficult, and barriers to implementation are many, including costs, time, patient health literacy, and patient and caregiver capacity to organize the regimen. Addressing these potential barriers with the patient can help to forge a therapeutic bond and may improve adherence; ignoring them will likely lead to therapeutic failure. Numerous studies demonstrate that, even in high-functioning health systems, only ~50% of patients are taking recommended, evidence-based secondary prevention medications, such as statins, by 1 year after a myocardial infarction.

In patients who are eligible for primary prevention strategies, it is important to frame the discussion around the overall evidence base as well as an individual patient’s likelihood of benefit from a given preventive intervention. A first step is to understand the patient’s estimated absolute risk for disease in the foreseeable future, or during their remaining lifespan. However, absolute risk estimation and presentation of those risks is generally insufficient to motivate behavior change. It is critical to assess the patient’s understanding and tolerance of the risk, their readiness to implement lifestyle changes or adhere to drug therapy, and their overall preferences regarding use of drug therapy to prevent an event (e.g., cancer, myocardial infarction, stroke). The clinician can help the patient by informing them of the risks for disease and potential for absolute benefits (and harms) from the available evidence-based choices. This may take more than one conversation, but given that diseases, such as cancer and cardiovascular disease, are the leading causes of premature death and disability, the time is well spent.

Partnering with the patient through motivational interviewing may assist in the process of selecting initial approaches to prevention. Selecting an area that the patient feels they are ready to change can lead to better adherence and greater achievement of success in the short and longer term. If the patient is uncertain what course to choose, prudence would dictate focusing on control of risk factors that may lead to the most rapid reduction in risk for acute events. For example, blood pressure is both a chronic risk factor and an acute trigger for cardiovascular events. Thus, if a patient has both significant elevations in blood pressure and dyslipidemia, it would be appropriate to focus initial efforts on blood pressure control. Likewise, focus on smoking cessation can lead to more rapid reductions in risk for acute events than some other lifestyle interventions.

PREVENTION AND HEALTH PROMOTION ACROSS THE LIFE COURSE
Periodic Health Evaluations
The “routine annual physical” has in many ways become an expected part of the patient-physician relationship in primary care practice. However, evidence for the efficacy of the periodic health evaluation in asymptomatic adults unselected for risk factors or disease is mixed, and depends on the outcome. Systematic reviews and meta-analyses of published trials have consistently observed lack of benefit (and also lack of harm) in terms of total mortality in association with periodic health evaluations. Data are more heterogeneous but overall suggest no benefit for cancer- or cardiovascular-specific mortality, with the potential for either benefit or harm depending on number of evaluations and patient-level factors. Well-designed studies on non-fatal clinical events and morbidity have been sparsely reported but there appear to be no large effects.

Periodic health evaluations do appear to lead to greater diagnosis of certain conditions such as hypertension and dyslipidemia, as expected. Likewise, periodic health examinations also improve the delivery of recommended preventive services, such as gynecologic examinations and Papanicolaou smears, fecal occult blood testing, and cholesterol screening. The benefits and risks associated with screening tests are discussed in detail in Chap. 4. Risks of routine evaluations include inappropriate or over-testing, or false-positive findings that require follow-up and induce patients to worry. Periodic health examinations appear to be associated with less patient worry. On balance, given the lack of convincing evidence of harm and the potential for better delivery of appropriate screening, counseling, and preventive services, periodic health evaluations appear reasonable for general populations at average risk for chronic conditions.

It is important to note that routine annual comprehensive physical examinations of asymptomatic adult patients have very low yield and may take an inordinate amount of time in a wellness visit. Such time may be better spent on assessing and counseling the patient on other aspects of their health, as discussed below. Evidence-based components that should be included in periodic evaluations focused on health and prevention include a number of age-appropriate screening tests for chronic disease and risk factors, preventive interventions including immunizations and chemoprevention for at-risk individuals, and preventive counseling. The United States Preventive Services Task Force publishes its Guide to Clinical Preventive Services, which contains evidence-based recommendations from the Task Force on preventive services for which there is a high degree of certainty that the service provides at least moderate net clinical benefit (i.e., benefits outweigh harms significantly and to a reasonable magnitude).

Healthy Behaviors and Lifestyles
Owing to the paucity of evidence, the heterogeneity of study designs and the diverse nature of interventions studied, many clinicians are uncertain as to how to deliver advice regarding healthy behaviors and lifestyles. Nevertheless, adverse behaviors and lifestyles contribute to more than 75% of premature, preventable deaths and disability. Estimates from the US National Health and Nutrition Survey indicate that fewer than 1% of Americans achieve an optimal heart-healthy eating pattern. Thus, whereas there are many demands on time during a typical patient-clinician encounter, few things may have more impact on longevity, health and quality of life for asymptomatic patients than an efficient approach to assessing, documenting, and improving patients’ health behaviors. Indeed, the mere act of assessing health behaviors has been shown to affect patient’s health behaviors. Facility with tools for assessment of lifestyle and with strategies for counseling are therefore of paramount importance.

Healthy Eating Patterns
Despite the existence of numerous “fad” diets, and seemingly inconsistent recommendations on dietary composition, there is remarkable agreement about what should constitute a healthy eating pattern for the broad population to avoid nutritional deficits (i.e., vitamin deficiency) and excesses (i.e., excessive caloric intake) and to maximize potential health (Table 2-1, see Chap. 325). Optimal eating patterns consist of whole fruits and vegetables, whole grains, lean proteins, healthy oils, and allow for non-fat or low-fat dairy intake. They tend to exclude frequent ingestion of foods high in refined sugars and starches, saturated fat, and sodium. Since sodium and refined sugars and starches are the hallmark of much of the processed/packaged food supply, a simple rule of thumb is to provide/cook the majority of one’s own meals starting from whole foods and emphasizing fruits and vegetables. Likewise, foods prepared outside of the home tend to have higher fat and sodium content, so special attention to menu choices focused on fruits, vegetables, lean proteins, and whole grains, while minimizing sauces and dressings can help most individuals follow healthier eating patterns. In all cases, sugar-sweetened beverages and non-nutritious snack foods should be minimized. If snacks are included, small amounts of healthy nuts and seeds, or more fruits and vegetables, should be encouraged.

TABLE 2-1
Guidelines and Key Recommendations from the Dietary Guidelines for Americans, 2015–2020
View Table|  Favorite Table|Download (.pdf)
Specific conditions and diseases, such as diabetes, other metabolic disorders, allergies, and gastrointestinal disorders, may require tailored approaches to diet. In counseling most patients, the general approach should focus on whole foods, eating patterns and appropriate calorie balance, rather than on specific micronutrients such as electrolytes or selected vitamins. It should be remembered that most patients have difficulty understanding nutritional labels on packaged foods, with the attendant demands on numeracy and health literacy.

Dietary guidelines are published by the US Department of Agriculture (USDA) and US Department of Health and Human Services every 5 years, and these guidelines have undergone substantial evolution over time. The current US Dietary Guidelines and Key Recommendations for 2015–2020 are summarized in Table 2-1 and emphasize eating patterns with nutrient-dense (rather than calorie-dense) whole foods, and appropriate caloric intake to achieve and maintain healthy weight. The USDA Guidelines focus on the concept of a healthy plate (rather than the prior food pyramid) for ease of counseling and adoption. Fifty percent of the plate should consist of fruits and vegetables, with remaining portions for whole grains and lean protein foods. When using fat for cooking, it should be done by sauteing in healthier oils (e.g., canola oil), and addition of judicious amounts of healthy raw oils (e.g., olive oil) to dishes is appropriate.

The USDA Guidelines focus on specific healthy eating patterns that adhere to these broad recommendations, and are appropriate for ~97% of the general population. They identify a “Healthy US-Style Eating Pattern” that adheres closely to the evidence-based Dietary Approaches to Stop Hypertension (DASH) eating pattern. Alternative patterns, which vary more in emphasis than in content, include a “Healthy Mediterranean-Style Eating Pattern” and a “Healthy Vegetarian Eating Pattern.”

AGE- AND SEX-SPECIFIC RECOMMENDATIONS
Current dietary recommendations are generally similar for all life stages from ages ≥2 years, but recommended levels of caloric intake (and hence amounts of foods) differ by age, sex, and physical activity level. For example, recommended caloric intake ranges from 1000 calories/d for sedentary 2-year-old children to as high as 3200 calories/d for active 16- to 18-year-old young men. Recommended caloric intakes peak in the early twenties for men and women and gradually decrease over ensuing decades.

As with all lifestyle counseling aimed at behavior change, dietary approaches that partner with the patient and utilize motivational interviewing strategies and shared goals and commitments tend to work best, as described below (see Approach to the Patient).

Physical Activity
Similar to the approach to counseling regarding healthy eating patterns, recommendations on participation in physical activity emphasize the point that any physical activity is better than none. A simple rule of thumb for patients is: “If you are doing nothing, do something; and if you are doing something, do more, every day.” The evidence base for physical activity indicates that the marginal benefits from physical activity are greatest in advancing from no activity to low levels of moderate activity. With increasing duration and intensity of activity, there is a continued curvilinear increase in health benefits, but the marginal gains for each additional minute of moderate-to-vigorous activity slowly diminish. Thus, for adults, the optimal amount of physical activity recommended is 150 min of moderate-intensity or 75 min of vigorous intensity aerobic activity per week, performed in episodes of at least 10 min, and preferably spread throughout the week. Additional health benefits can be realized by engaging in physical activity beyond this amount, and/or by adding muscle-strengthening activities that involve all major muscle groups 2 or more days per week.

In counseling patients regarding physical activity, it is important to note that sedentary time (e.g., seated at work, or at home in front of electronic screens) has adverse health consequences independent of the lack of physical activity during these episodes. Therefore, even modest efforts like standing at the desk and doing gentle stretching for periods during the day may be beneficial. It is also important to emphasize that participating in a variety of aerobic activities (biking, swimming, walking, jogging, rowing, elliptical training, stair-climbing, etc.) can be beneficial and may help to avoid overuse injuries and boredom with the exercise regimen. If patients choose to participate in muscle-strengthening activities for health improvement, emphasis should be placed on weights that allow more repetitions (e.g., 3 sets of 15–20 repetitions that can be performed comfortably, with a rest period in between) and on avoiding breath-holding and straining against a closed glottis.

SUDDEN CARDIAC DEATH RISK
Patients may express concerns regarding the risk of sudden cardiac death during exercise. Whereas the risk of sudden death during exercise does increase directly with the amount of time spent exercising, this association is substantially mitigated by training effects. Thus, patients embarking on an exercise program should be encouraged to increase the duration of aerobic exercise gradually as tolerated, aiming for episodes of at least 30 min 5 times a week as an ideal. Once a comfortable duration is reached, incorporating interval training periods of more intensive activity interspersed during the exercise can provide greater fitness gains.

EXTREME ENDURANCE ACTIVITIES
As with other forms of exercise, extreme endurance activities such as triathlons and marathons should be undertaken only with appropriate and graded training. Such activities tend to take a greater toll on the musculoskeletal system over time than less extreme activities, and they are also associated with measurable damage to the myocardium and greater risks for other organ damage. Athletes participating in endurance activities routinely have elevations in cardiac troponin (a specific circulating marker of myocardial cell damage and death) at the end of the race, although elevations are lower in those who are well trained. Patients and clinicians should consider the patient’s overall health, specific limitations, potential for injury, and ability to train in decision-making regarding participation in endurance events.

AGE-SPECIFIC RECOMMENDATIONS
The US Department of Health and Human Services’ Physical Activity Guidelines for Americans (Table 2-2) recommend that children and adolescents aged 6–17 years should participate in ≥60 min of physical activity daily, most of which should be moderate- or vigorous-intensity aerobic activity, including vigorous activity at least 3 days a week. As noted above, adults aged 18–64 years are recommended to pursue at least 150 min of moderate-intensity or 75 min of vigorous-intensity aerobic activity per week (or equivalent combinations). Adults aged ≥65 years should follow the adult guidelines, or be as active as possible as abilities and conditions allow. Special emphasis is also placed on exercises to improve balance in those at risk for falling.

TABLE 2-2
Recommendations from Physical Activity Guidelines for Americans
View Table|  Favorite Table|Download (.pdf)
Sleep Hygiene
Sleeping between 7 and 9 h per night appears to be optimal for health in adults aged ≥18 years. Sleeping <7 9="" achieving="" adolescents="" adverse="" all-cause="" among="" and="" appropriate="" are="" as="" associated="" better="" blood="" but="" cardiovascular="" children="" cognitive="" control="" conversely="" depression="" deprivation="" diabetes="" disease="" disturbances="" effects="" elevated="" for="" from="" function="" h="" health="" hypertension="" illness="" immune="" impaired="" improved="" in="" including="" increased="" individuals="" is="" levels="" loss="" mental="" more="" mortality="" most="" night="" obesity="" of="" on="" or="" outcomes="" p="" pain="" patients="" per="" performance.="" physiologic="" pressure="" recovering="" regular="" sensitivity="" sleep="" success="" such="" than="" the="" uncertain.="" weight="" well="" with="">
Patients often express concerns about the quantity and quality of their sleep. With aging, both aspects of sleep tend to decline, even without overt sleep disorders. Documentation of sleep using a sleep log may assist in understanding different types of insomnia and sleep disorders. Encouraging daily activity to promote fatigue, avoidance of eating and drinking alcohol too close to bedtime, and regular daily sleep habits may help patients achieve better sleep. Regular use of sedative medications should generally be discouraged given the high potential for dependence, addiction, and altered sleep quality.

DISORDERS OF SLEEP
The prevalence of sleep-related breathing disorders, including obstructive sleep apnea (OSA), is poorly documented. Based on data from the 1990s, the prevalence of diagnosed mild OSA in the US population was ~10%, and of moderate to severe apnea was ~5%. However, the increasing prevalence of obesity, a major risk factor for OSA, suggests that the prevalence may have increased. The prevalence of asymptomatic or undiagnosed sleep apnea is unknown. Patients with persistent complaints of poor sleep quality, excessive daytime somnolence, or with witnessed apneic spells may benefit from screening for sleep disorders, prior to consideration of a formal sleep study. A number of clinical tools have been developed to screen for sleep apnea, including the Epworth Sleepiness Scale, the STOP (Snoring, Tiredness, Observed apnea, high blood Pressure) Questionnaire, and the STOP-Bang Questionnaire (STOP plus assessment of body mass index, age, neck circumference, and gender), among others. The US Preventive Services Task Force found that current evidence is insufficient to assess the balance of benefits and harms of screening for OSA in asymptomatic adults owing to a lack of validation data in primary care settings. Nonetheless, the high prevalence and significant health consequences of sleep apnea suggest that clinicians should be alert for its potential presence, particularly in patients who are obese with symptoms of excessive daytime somnolence or witnessed apnea episodes. Other sleep disorders, such as restless leg syndrome, may be identified with simple history.

Weight Management
Overweight and obesity are prevalent in epidemic proportions in the US and other industrialized nations (Chaps. 394, 395). Since 1985, the prevalence of obesity in the United States has increased from ~10% to almost 35%, and the prevalence of overweight is now ~40%. Overweight and obesity disproportionately affect individuals in lower socio-economic strata, and in many underserved minority populations, including African Americans, Latino Americans, and American Indians. In all race-ethnic groups, both overweight and obesity are associated with adverse health consequences, including diabetes, certain cancers, cardiovascular diseases, and degenerative joint disease. Eating disorders such as anorexia and bulimia are much less common but pose major health consequences for affected patients, and should be suspected particularly in younger women with history of rapid weight shifts or underweight status.

Weight loss is one of the most difficult preventive interventions to achieve and sustain over time. However, several key factors can assist the patient and clinician, and early referral to a dietician can be very helpful. The first therapeutic goal is to aim for weight stabilization. Many of the risks of overweight and obesity are driven more strongly by continued weight gain, rather than overweight/obese status per se. Working with the patient to find initial strategies for weight maintenance can be a successful initial step with success for many patients. For those who can progress to considering weight loss, it is critical to help the patient understand that there is no standard solution. Experimentation and documentation are key. Tools to assist patients can include food and weight logs, activity logs, and smart phone apps. Some patients respond best to structured commercial dietary programs where meals are provided to them. Any of these approaches can be tried with or without social group supports.

The key construct for weight loss is, of course, negative calorie balance. This is achieved through a combination of reduced caloric intake and increased physical activity. Patients may already understand, from prior weight loss attempts, what combination works best for them to achieve this. Some patients find that they cannot lose weight without increasing their exercise. For many, reduction of caloric intake is most efficient. Encouraging the patient to find what works for them is most important. The same principle holds for dietary content. Well done feeding studies indicate that weigh loss is dependent far more on the reduction of caloric intake than on the relative composition of fat, protein and carbohydrate in the diet. There may be other medical reasons to choose one approach over another, but if not, encouraging the patient to pick one approach and document the results is an important start.

Tobacco Cessation
Escaping nicotine dependence is another major, but critical, challenge to prevention and wellness efforts (see Chap. 448). The addictive effects of nicotine have been well documented, with effects that can last for years after successful cessation. Assessing a patient’s past history of cessation attempts and current readiness for change are key first steps in forging a successful approach. Frequent follow-up and reinforcement, as well as use of nicotine replacement therapy and other cessation-promoting medications are additional critical elements. Recidivism is the rule, and patients should expect to resume smoking and attempt again as they journey to tobacco cessation.

MENTAL HEALTH AND ADDICTION
Assessment for depression and cognitive impairment are important to address when patients exhibit symptoms, or they or their family members express concerns. Both of these common conditions play a major role in reducing quality of life and are high on patients’ lists of concerns, even if not clearly expressed. Screening tools for depression are reviewed in Chap. 444. Cognitive function decline with aging or comorbid illness, including depression, should be anticipated. Assessment tools such as the General Practitioner Assessment of Cognition or the Mini-CogTM test are widely available and effective rapid assessment tools.

Alcohol and Opioids
Alcohol dependence and abuse are common and underdiagnosed (see Chaps. 445, 446). Rapid screening tools have proven efficacy for identifying patients with alcohol problems. In a systematic review, the CAGE (cut down, guilty, annoyed, eye opener) questionnaire was most effective at identifying alcohol abuse and dependence, with reasonable sensitivity and high specificity. The present opioid epidemic in the United States presents a new and substantial public health challenge given the high potential for dependency and abuse of these drugs. Rapid screening tools are being developed and validated to assist clinicians in screening for opioid dependence.

ACCIDENTS AND SUICIDE
Regular assessment of patient safety through simple questions about seat belt use, domestic violence, and gun safety in the home continue to be important parts of health promotion and wellness. Longstanding recommendations for assessment of suicidal ideation among patients with depression or a history of suicide attempts also continue to be relevant.

APPROACH TO THE PATIENT
APPROACH TO THE PATIENT
In the context of a clinical visit focused on health assessment, health promotion, and prevention, the basic skills of history taking are of paramount importance. Much of the evaluation, counseling, and management that focus on health promotion and prevention also require engagement and buy-in from the patient in order to assist with recognition of contributing behaviors and to promote adherence to therapeutic plans. Therefore, in addition to standard history-taking, additional skills such as motivational interviewing and eliciting patient commitments and contracting may prove of significant value. The availability of additional tools to assist with screening and chronic management, both online and through mobile health technologies, is rapidly expanding, with uncertain implications for the future. Major research gaps exist in our understanding of how best to employ these newer technologies to improve health outcomes. Concepts of behavioral economics are being explored to better understand the psychology of decision-making and incentives as a means to improve lifestyle choices and adherence to treatment plans (Chap. 468).

The limited time available to clinicians and patients during a wellness visit or periodic health examination (not driven by specific patient issues) makes it important to prioritize assessment and counseling for factors that affect longevity, healthspan, and quality of life over approaches that may have low yield, such as the annual comprehensive physical examination in an asymptomatic patient. Setting clear expectations for the content of a wellness visit may be a first step, and scheduling follow-up visits for findings or to continue indicated counseling are important steps to achieving better health outcomes.